ľֱ

Here's How to Think About the Danish Mask Study

— What the DANMASK-19 trial showed and didn't show about mask use and COVID-19

MedpageToday
The flag of Denmark printed on a protective mask

I was drawn to science and medicine because of all human endeavors, ours is one where smart people can say, "I don't know." We don't stop there. We run a study or experiment that helps us know more. That is what makes what we do different than other human endeavors.

I start then with a humility as I take on the Danish Mask Study, in the Annals of Internal Medicine. This is a 4,800-person randomized trial that took place in the spring and early summer in Denmark. The trial was run at a time where most Danes did not wear masks when they left their house. It told participants to practice social distancing and randomly assigned them with the advice to wear a mask (and even gave them 50 surgical masks), advising them to change it after every 8 hours of use, or gave them no advice to wear mask, and followed them to see how many acquired SARS-CoV-2 by PCR or antibody testing. The answer was a nearly identical proportion -- 42 of 2,393 people (1.8%) in the mask group and 53 of 2,470 (2.1%) in the no-mask group. The difference was not statistically significant.

Before I tell you what the study showed and didn't, we have to consider some criticism that is rapidly emerging.

Was the trial underpowered? The trial was powered to test its hypothesis of a 50% reduction in SARS-CoV-2 from mask wearing in a setting where the baseline risk was approximately 2%. The trial anticipated a 20% loss to follow-up. By these measures the trial was adequately powered to test its hypothesis, but let's be honest, the authors could not possibly have known at the outset the exact rate of COVID-19. While 2% was a terrific guess, it so easily could have been 22%. SARS-CoV-2 is a fat-tail probability event: meaning that it is possible for extremely bad scenarios to occur. Instead of California, Denmark could have been South Dakota! For this reason, I don't judge them harshly for power.

Was adherence poor? Among participants, 46% wore the mask as recommended and 47% wore it "predominantly as recommended," for a total of 93%. Anyone who has walked around any city or store in America can attest: that is actually pretty good! To my eye, one in four noses are seen protruding, and one in eight masks are worn as a chin strap.

Even low adherence would not be a problem, as it's part of what is being tested. A cake is both the batter and the temperature of the oven, and a public health recommendation is both the recommendation itself and whether people follow it. You should be judged for both things.

What does the trial show exactly? The Danish trial shows that this specific mask recommendation (plus a box of masks) made during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic, with background rates of 2% PCR acquisition, failed to show that mask wearing reduces risk by 50%. In places where there is modest SARS-CoV-2 transmission (like Denmark during these months), there is insufficient evidence to suggest wearing a mask as you go about daily errands will protect you from infection. That is good to know!

What doesn't the trial show? The trial is not able to assess the claim, "My mask protects you, not me." The way to test this claim would be to randomize clusters or groups of people. Perhaps by city or county, and ask if mask mandates slow spread across all folks who live in that area. To my knowledge there has never been such a study, and while this message is popular and plausible, we should be willing to say, "I don't know for sure if it is true." By the way, we have done so many cluster randomized controlled trials in medicine, .

What the trial really means? Above all else, the Danish trial . We need these studies now more than ever. Let's be honest. Masks have become a hot-button political issue. They are increasingly a badge symbolizing who one voted for. This is a terrible consequence of bad leadership and caustic, polarized social media posts -- yes, unfortunately, by both proponents and opponents of masks.

The trials we need right now are cluster randomized trials to test messaging strategies. Is SARS-CoV-2 transmission slowed in counties/cities where we (a) advise people to wear a cloth mask because it's the patriot thing to do (b) advise them to wear a cloth mask because it may protect others (c) advise people to wear surgical masks and distribute households a box (d) advise masks indoors only, but no need to wear a mask outdoors, or (e) no additional comments made by officials. There are many important testable questions.

Should the trial have been published? Some have turned to social media to ask why a trial that may diminish enthusiasm for masks and may be misinterpreted was published in a top medical journal. Woah! First, of all, I am prepared to die on the hill that science means publishing the results of truthful experiments no matter what they show. We can acknowledge limits, but we can never suppress results. Second, in today's environment of rampant conspiracy theories, watching doctors openly discuss not publishing results seems to be ... I can't think of a polite word.

Don't make masks more political than they have become! Experts on twitter do not realize what they are doing. The more one's social media feed becomes a mix of posts claiming Trump is stupid, Biden is great, and masks are awesome, the more those become wedded together, as a single package deal. If one wishes to be a public health expert for all Americans, one cannot be a naked political actor online.

Why so much criticism? I think we have to consider why we have such strong emotions toward masks. Amongst all the non-pharmacologic things one can do to potentially slow the spread of SARS-CoV-2, masks dominate the conversation. Handwashing has faded entirely from the discussion. One possibility is that masks are a clear and visible symbol, and handwashing is invisible. We have to consider the fact that we are reacting to this study on an emotional level, and those emotions are also preventing us from doing additional trials (such as the cluster trials I describe above) that might yield useful insights.

Final thoughts? I have a few. Masks are not parachutes. Their effect size at best is going to be modest. Every time you muster the breath to advise someone to wear a mask, advise handwashing, distancing, and avoiding indoor gatherings too. Next, have some perspective on masks. Whether or not they work indoors with prolonged contact among adults is one thing, but they surely do nothing if children are outdoors playing in the rain. And yet, the state of Washington has mandated that youth sports wear masks when playing outside in the Seattle winter at all times. No one would even do a randomized trial of this strategy, as it is openly foolish. The moment the mask gets wet, it's useless and that is just one of the problems. Let us save our breath for the recommendations that matter.

In short, the Danish mask study doesn't answer every question, but it was well done, thoughtful, useful, and what we need more of. I commend Christine Laine, MD, MPH, and her fellow editors at the Annals of Internal Medicine for publishing it. The investigators will not get the praise they deserve, but I will praise them. They are scientists, and there are still a few of us left breathing who can recognize that.

, is a hematologist-oncologist and associate professor of medicine at the University of California San Francisco, and author of